Research Communications

OPEN  ACCESS

Using Ethnotaxonomy to Assess Traditional Knowledge and Language Vitality: A Case Study with the Vaie People of Sarawak, Malaysia

Syafitri Hidayati1, Bibi Aminah Abdul Ghani2, Beena Giridharan3, Mohd Zafri Hassan4, and F. Merlin Franco5, 6*

1Faculty of Engineering and Science, Curtin University Malaysia, Miri. 2Faculty of Language and Communication, University Malaysia Sarawak (UNIMAS), Kuching. 3 Office of the Pro Vice Chancellor, Curtin University Malaysia, Miri. 4Faculty of Agriculture and Food Sciences University Putra Malaysia Bintulu Campus, Bintulu. 5Curtin University Malaysia, Miri. 6Institute of Asian Studies, University Brunei Darussalam

*tropicalforezt@gmail.com

Received July 7, 2016 | Accepted September 11, 2017

Ethnobiology Letters 2018 9(2):33–47 | DOI 10.14237/ebl.9.2.2018.740

Abstract This article demonstrates the potential of using ethnotaxonomy and nomenclature to assess the vitality status of indigenous languages and traditional knowledge at the ecosystem level. We collaborated with the Vaie people of Sarawak, Malaysia, applying a mixed methodology approach that relies on free-listing to a large extent. We applied the Traditional Knowledge and Language Vitality (TraLaVi) index to assess traditional knowledge and language vitality against five major parameters, specifically: language priority, retrieval of information, knowledge erosion, lexical recognition, and social support for exchange of traditional knowledge.  The results show that with a TraLaVi score of 0.84, the Vaie language can be considered “safe”. Individuals practicing the traditional occupation of fishing fared better (mean=0.90) than those of the non-fishermen group (mean=0.77). However, when the language vitality was assessed using the Language Vitality and Endangerment assessment tool of UNESCO, the results indicate that the Vaie language could potentially be in the “unsafe” zone, highlighting the differences between the ecosystem based approach of the TraLaVi and the macro-approach of UNESCO. However, these approaches can be applied in a complementary manner to generate a more accurate portrayal of the language vitality scenario.

Keywords Folk names, Vernacular names, Language vitality and endangerment, Linguistic ethnobiology

One of the major contributions of ethnobiology was the conceptualization in the 1960s of a theoretical framework for folk taxonomy (Hunn 2007). Three decades later, the concept of Biocultural Diversity was born, coinciding with a renewed academic interest in the synergy between indigenous languages, traditional knowledge (TK), and biological diversity. This concept paved the way for a new wave of research that focused significantly on the use of vernacular/folk names, analyzing them both from a linguistic and TK perspective (Evans 1997; Kakudidi 2004; Turpin 2013; Unasho 2013; Zariquiey 2014). Researchers have demonstrated that folk names are not mere lexemes, but condensed forms of knowledge with multiple applications. Analyses of folk names have helped us understand how migrant populations adapt to their new ecosystems (van Andel et al. 2014), indicate linguistic stra­tigraphy (Bostoen 2007), solve important questions related to distribution of iconic trees (Rangan et al. 2015), and unravel mysteries of domestication of food plants (Donohue and Denham 2009). Drawing from a collaborative study conducted with the Vaie people of Sarawak in Malaysia, this article demonstrates an additional dimension of folk taxonomy: the possibility of using folk names to assess a community’s TK and language vitality simultaneously (Franco et al. 2015).

The Vaie People and Language

The Vaie people, popularly known as Ba’ie or Bintulu, speak a language known by various names such as Bintulu, Ba’ie, or Vaie (Asmah 1983; Ibrahim 1971). The community prefers to refer to themselves and their language as “Vaie”. Consequently, in this article the term “Vaie” is used to refer to both the community and their language. Traditionally, they practice a fishing system called panau where fishes such as Parastromateus niger, Atule mate, Carangoides praeustus, Carangoides armatus, and Carangoides coeruleopinnatus are trapped using a lure made from nipah leaves (Nypa fruticans Wurmb). Some researchers consider Vaie to be a variant of the Melanau language (Asmah 1983), though Blust (1974) and Zaini (1989) consider it to bear little similarities to other Melanau languages in the region. Ethnologue too lists the language as a distinct one under the name “Bintulu” (Simons and Fennig 2017), in concurrence with the emic consideration. Most of the Vaie people in Bintulu maintain a diglossic situation in which Malay and Vaie languages are used for different purposes. Besides native speakers, the Vaie language is also spoken by a small section of other indigenous communities in Bintulu. Today, the Vaie language is spoken in the kampung (villages) close to Bintulu town, namely: Kampung Masjid, Kampung Sinong, Kampung Datuk, Kampung Sibiew, Kampung Baru, Kampung Jepak, Kampung Sebuan, Kampung Batu Sepuluh, and Kidurung. According to the Department of Statistics, Malaysia, the total population of Bintulu in the year 2010 was 183,892, with Ibans comprising 42%, Chinese 21%, Melanau 12%, Malays 10%, and Bidayuh, Indian, Non-Malaysians and other indigenous groups 14%. It is highly possible that Vaie people were included under the Melanau group, bringing the population to an estimated 23,000. Our interviews with the Vaie people indicate that the population may in fact number only around 18,000, qualifying it as a “small” community as defined by Krauss (1991). The Alliance for Linguistic Diversity (2015) considers Vaie to be a “vulnerable” language (also see: Ghani 2006). 

Methodology

We assessed language priority (criterion A), adeptness in retrieving information in both the autochthonous language and allochthonous language (criterion B), knowledge erosion (criterion C), lexical recognition (criterion D), and social support for exchange of TK (criterion E) by applying the TraLaVi index developed by Franco et al. (2015). The study closely follows the methodology suggested by Franco et al. (2015), except that plants have been replaced by fishes in the present study. As the community is traditionally a fishing community, we assumed that knowledge related to fishes would be common to all members of the community, and community members who have drifted away from the traditional occupation of fishing could show decline in TK related to fishes. The fieldwork for the research was undertaken in December 2014–February 2015 in collaboration with the Vaie people of Kampung Kuala Tatau, Kampung Segan, Kampung Sebuan, Kampung Jepak, Kampung Batu 10, Kampung Sebiew, Kampung Baru, Kampung Dato, Kampung Sinong, and Kampung Masjid, Kidurong, and Kampung Asyakirin in the Bintulu region.

Ethical clearance for the study was obtained from the Curtin Ethics Committee (Ap­proval No. CSEA 041214, dated 4 December 2014), and a research permit was attained from the Sarawak State Planning Unit prior to the commencement of the study. Informed consent was obtained from each individual before the interview; the entire study conforms to the code of ethics of the International Society of Ethnobiology (2006). In addition, informal conversations were carried out with prominent individuals of the community to ensure that the methodology and outcome were culturally relevant, and reflected the community’s needs and concerns. Fourteen knowledgeable individuals aged 59 and above (male n= 8, female n= 6), selected on the basis of their reputation as traditional knowledge holders, were invited to participate in open-ended interviews to elicit baseline information on the Vaie culture, language, and TK. Vaie TK on fishes, including folk names and their meanings, culinary recipes, totems, taboos, ecology, and folklore were documented through in-depth interviews. We used the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA) to transcribe Vaie fish names following Ghani (1992), and the data were analyzed to develop an outline of Vaie ethnotaxonomy and its nomenclatural system.

In the second phase, 16 elders (male n=8, female n=8) above the age of sixty who did not participate in the previous phase were randomly selected and invited to free-list 25 fish names. This participant group was limited to only Vaie people with Vaie parents and Vaie grandparents, to conform to the cultural definition of “Vaie” prevailing within the community. From the interviews, 25 final candidate fishes were shortlisted on the basis of salience (Table 1). Open-ended conversations were carried out after the focused interviews to elicit TK on all fishes known to the Vaie community. The shortlisted fishes were identified scientifically using field guides and identification sheets (Khiok and Ali 2014; Khiok and Gambang 2009). The primary author then accompanied community members to various fish landing sites and local markets to collect specimens and to photograph the identified fishes; these photographs were later used as visual stimuli for criterion D.

The third phase involved interviewing members of the younger generation identified through snowball sampling (Luborsky and Rubinstein 1995). Since the community practiced fishing traditionally, fishing was considered a key indicator of their culture and the participants in this phase were divided into two clusters: Cluster 1 was comprised of participants who were involved in the traditional profession of fishing (n=30, 20–50 years); Cluster 2 (n=30, 20–50 years) was comprised of participants who were not involved in fishing as a profession. Cluster 1 was comprised of 15 males who practiced fishing and 15 female participants whose husbands were fishermen. Culturally, Vaie women are not involved in fishing, yet they play an important supportive role in helping their husbands to grade fish according to their quality, and in converting them into value-added products. Cluster 2 included five individuals who practiced fishing as a hobby, as well as their wives, in addition to people who did not practice fishing. Participants were requested to answer a simple questionnaire for assessing language proficiency. The questions were:

(1) What is your first language (L1)?

(2) How many languages do you speak?

(3) What is your second language (L2)?

(4) My proficiency in Vaie language is*…

(5) My proficiency in (L2) is*…

*Note that questions 4 and 5 used a five point Likert scale: very poor (1), poor (2), moderate (3), good (4), and very good (5). 

Subsequently, semi-structured interviews were also conducted to elicit information re­quired to calculate the Traditional Knowledge and Language Vitality Index (TraLaVi), and results were tabulated following Franco et al. (2015) for analysis. During the interviews, participants free-listed fish names both in L1 and L2; the time taken for free-listing was noted following Franco et al. (2015). In addition, the results of the language proficiency questionnaire used by the participants were correlated with Criteria A of the TraLaVi table to understand if the participants’ self-assessment reflected the actual extent of priority accorded to L1. Criterion C of the TraLaVi assesses participants’ ability to interpret vernacular names, which becomes problematic in the case of fishes denoted by unanalyzable lexemes; participants with sound language and TK would not be able to provide the meaning for such names. To overcome this, a full rating was given for such lexemes provided that participants clearly identify the lexeme as “unanalyzable”.  While the values for the TraLaVi table indicate the overall vitality status of the community’s language and TK, sub-analyses of the clusters and the genders provide insights into the intra-communal dynamics of the language and TK. To provide a comparative outlook of the linguistic vitality scenario, the TraLaVi values were then compared against that of the Language Vitality and Endangerment (LVE index) developed by the UNESCO Ad Hoc Group on Endangered Languages, in 2003. Further, an open-ended interview consisting of leads meant to elicit information required for the nine factors listed by the LVE was carried out with the 60 respondents who had participated in the TraLaVi interviews. These results were also compared with that of TraLaVi.

Results and Discussion

As a fishing community, the Vaie people regard fish as being culturally significant. All 25 fishes salient in the community are either fried, steamed, made into curry, or smoked/ salted for preserving. Notable mentions were njen tengiriq (Scomberomorus commerson and Scomberomorus guttatus) consumed as a tonic/vitalizer by new mothers, njen tavai (Wallago leerii) that is featured prominently in folklore connected to the origin of the Vaie people, and njen gilau (Clarias nieuhofii) and njen seqael (Plotosus canius), which are considered toxic, thus requiring detoxification before consumption.  A noteworthy feature of the shortlisted fish names is that the majority (20/25) of the lexemes used to denote the fishes were unanalyzable. The results of the study are presented in Tables 1–3.

 

Table 1 Twenty-five culturally salient fishes of the Vaie people.

No.

Vernacu­lar Name

Scientific Name

Uses

Meaning of Vernacular Names

1

Njen ruay

Parastromateus niger 

(Bloch, 1795)

Sold fresh, unaffordable fish around 20-35 MYR/ kg. It is given three names according to its life stages and size. Used to prepare umai raway. The stomach is used to prepare tagik (preserved in glass bottle).

Unanalyzable

2

Njen tengiriq

Scomberomorus
 commerson

  (Lacepède, 1800)

Sold fresh, smoked, or salted. Commonly fried, cooked with turmeric, or as curry. Preparation of pipos that is consumed by new mothers. 

Unanalyzable

 

 

Scomberomorus 
guttatus 

(Bloch & Schneider, 1801)

3

Njen jamah

Atule mate  (Cuvier, 1833)

Sold fresh. Caught using the panau traditional fishing technique. Preparation of umai. One of the favorite fishes of Vaie.

Refers to the Carangidae group

4

Njen puqoq

Otolithoides biauritus 
(Cantor, 1849)

Sold fresh, dried or salted. Favorite fish; dried or salted, fried or cooked as curry.

Unanalyzable

5

Njen buleng

Nemapteryx macronotacantha (Bleeker1846)

Sold fresh and smoked. Has a corrupted name njen proton saga.

Unanalyzable

6

Njen piras

Setipinna breviceps (Cantor, 1849)

Sold fresh. Preparation of umai. The most favorite fish for making umai. Sometimes also fried.

Unanalyzable

7

Njen pay

Neotrygon kuhlii 
 
(Müller & Henle, 1841)

Sold fresh or salted. Heart of the fish is highly priced. Commonly cooked as curry, roasted, or as masak sambal.

Unanalyzable

8

Qeret

Carcharhinus 
amblyrhynchos 
(Whiteley, 1934)

Sold fresh. Preparation of umai, commonly cooked as curry and soups. Sometimes roasted without oil since the fish is oily.

Unanalyzable

9

Njen seqael

Plotosus canius  
(Hamilton, 1822)

Sold fresh. It is toxic and requires treatment before cooking. Commonly cooked with coconut milk, curry, or masak sambal.

Unanalyzable

10

Njen lata’

Lobotes surinamensis 
 
(Bloch, 1790)

Sold fresh. Head is the favorite part, commonly cooked as curry or spicy-sour curry. Some people also like to roast the fish.

Unanalyzable

11

Njen gagog

Arius sp.

Sold fresh and smoked. Has a corrupted name njen proton saga.

Unanalyzable

12

Njen re­man

Rastrel­liger kanagurta

  (Cuvier, 1816)

Sold fresh or salted. Commonly fried. Abundant and always available in markets.

Unanalyzable

 

 

 

Rastrelliger brachysoma  (Bleeker, 1851)

13

Njen taoq

Osteogeneiosus militaris (Linnaeus, 1758)

Sold fresh or smoked.

Unanalyzable

14

Njen tavai

Wallago leerii  
(Bleeker, 1851)

Sold fresh. Commonly cooked inside bamboo (pansuh). Appears in the folklore connected with the origin of Segan.

Unanalyzable

15

Njen bageng

Arius maculatus (Thunberg, 1792)

Sold fresh and smoked, has a corrupted name njen proton saga.

Unanalyzable

16

Njen bibeq

Pampus argenteus (Euphrasen, 1788)

Sold fresh. Commonly fried for consumption.

Unanalyzable

17

Njen da’ie

Kryptopterus kryptopterus (Bleeker, 1851)

Sold fresh. Commonly cooked without gut due to the high fecal content. Considered as a favorite fish of Chinese.

Da’ie= ta’ie = feces; the fish feeds on feces

18

Njen kelapa

Lactarius lactarius 
 
(Bloch & Schneider, 1801)

Sold fresh or dried. Abundant and always available in market. Commonly fried or cooked with turmeric.

Kelapa= coconut; fish is as white as coconut meat.

19

Njen selusong

Lates calcarifer (Bloch, 1790)

Sold fresh. Unaffordable fish around 35 MYR/kg. Usually steamed, head preferred and cooked as curry.

Unanalyzable

20

Njen terupbuk

Tenualosa toli (Valenciennes, 1847)

Sold fresh or salted following Kuching culture. Usually fried.

Unanalyzable

21

Njen bengetot

Ilisha pristigastroides (Bleeker 1852)

Sold fresh or dried and salted. Usually made into the pickle “masak sambal” and roasted. Makes a sound “tod” when caught.

tot”= sound tot; the fish produces a “tot” sound when caught.

22

Njen gilau

Clarias nieuhofii  
(Valenciennes, 1840)

Sold fresh and usually fried, cooked with coconut milk, and masak sambal. It is mildly toxic and has to be detoxified before consumption

Unanalyzable

23

Njen qapaw

Epinephelus 
sexfasciatus 
 
(Valenciennes, 1828)

Sold fresh. Fish with one of the highest price tags; in huge demand for seafood restaurants. Commonly prepared as masak sambal, curry, or fried.

Unanalyzable

 

 

 

Cephalopholis boenak 

 (Bloch, 1790)

 

 

Epinephelus areolatus 

 (Forsskål, 1775)

24

Njen tuqol

Thunnus tonggol 
(Bleeker, 1851)

Sold fresh or smoked. Usually fried, cooked with turmeric, cooked as curry or with coconut milk. Abundant and always available in market.

Unanalyzable

25

Njen alu-alu

Sphyraena barracuda 
 
(Edwards, 1771)

Sold fresh. Usually cooked as curry or with coconut milk.

Alu-alu = rice pestle; the fish is cylin­drical and long as the pestle used to pound rice.

 

On the basis of the average value obtained, the TraLaVi scale categorizes language and TK into dead (0), moribund (0.1–0.25), endangered (0.25–0.5), vulnerable (0.5–0.75), and safe (0.75–1).   With an average value of 0.84, the Vaie language can be deemed in the “safe” category on the TraLaVi scale (Table 2). This indicates that the Vaie people have been adept in balancing their proficiency in L1 and L2 while at the same time maintaining their TK. In general, individuals from the fishermen group C1 did better (mean= 0.90) than those who belonged to the non-fishermen group, C2 (mean= 0.77). However, with a mean score of 0.77, the language and TK vitality of the non-fishermen group is only slightly above the score of 0.75 that would indicate a “vulnerable” status as per the TraLaVi scale.  The results of the study can be further compartmentalized as below.

 

Table 2Traditional Knowledge and Language Vitality of Vaie people.

Pa

Cluster

Sex

Proficiency

Time (s)

Criteria

TraLaVi Score

L1b

L2c

L1

L2

A

B

C

D

E

1

1

M

5

5

219

269

25

24

24

23

23

0.952

2

1

M

5

5

217

452

25

24

23

23

23

0.944

3

1

M

5

5

196

537

25

25

25

23

23

0.968

4

1

M

5

3

261

303

25

24

24

24

24

0.968

5

1

M

5

3

189

377

25

24

24

23

23

0.952

6

1

M

5

3

276

562

25

25

25

24

24

0.984

7

1

M

5

5

604

602

25

21

21

23

23

0.904

8

1

M

5

5

249

427

25

25

25

24

24

0.984

9

1

M

5

4

225

871

25

24

24

23

23

0.952

10

1

M

5

5

240

256

25

22

22

20

20

0.872

11

1

M

5

5

233

335

25

23

23

17

17

0.840

12

1

M

5

3

329

1050

25

21

21

19

19

0.840

13

1

M

5

5

114

233

25

22

22

17

17

0.824

14

1

M

5

4

128

267

25

25

25

21

21

0.936

15

1

M

5

4

147

150

25

24

24

25

25

0.984

16

1

F

5

5

267

332

25

25

25

22

22

0.952

17

1

F

5

5

161

338

25

24

24

21

21

0.920

18

1

F

5

5

178

234

25

24

24

20

20

0.904

19

1

F

5

5

905

745

25

23

23

19

19

0.872

20

1

F

5

5

360

891

25

25

25

20

20

0.920

21

1

F

5

5

334

863

25

25

25

18

18

0.888

22

1

F

5

5

303

189

25

24

24

21

21

0.920

23

1

F

5

5

224

180

25

24

24

18

18

0.872

24

1

F

5

5

283

319

25

24

24

17

17

0.856

25

1

F

5

5

385

413

25

22

22

16

16

0.808

26

1

F

5

5

233

315

25

24

24

18

18

0.872

27

1

F

5

4

365

716

25

23

23

16

16

0.824

28

1

F

5

5

114

120

25

22

22

13

13

0.760

29

1

F

5

3

211

Gave up

25

24

24

20

20

0.904

30

1

F

5

3

236

Gave up

25

22

22

15

15

0.792

31

2*

M

5

3

447

861

25

23

23

21

21

0.904

32

2*

M

5

5

199

462

25

24

24

23

23

0.952

33

2*

M

5

5

220

308

25

24

24

22

22

0.936

34

2*

M

5

5

447

867

25

23

23

22

22

0.920

35

2*

M

5

5

207

209

25

25

25

23

23

0.968

36

2

M

5

5

259

242

15

23

23

20

20

0.808

37

2

M

5

5

200

252

25

21

21

6

6

0.632

38

2

M

5

5

233

180

15

23

23

15

15

0.728

39

2

M

5

5

772

347

0

20

20

13

12

0.520

40

2

M

5

5

368

316

15

23

23

13

12

0.688

41

2

M

5

5

247

293

25

25

25

19

19

0.904

42

2

M

5

3

364

335

15

25

25

15

0

0.640

43

2

M

5

5

364

335

15

25

25

15

15

0.760

44

2

M

5

5

262

618

25

24

24

20

20

0.904

45

2

M

5

4

309

154

0

22

22

20

20

0.672

46

2

F

5

5

181

334

25

24

24

24

24

0.968

47

2

F

5

5

675

304

0

24

24

17

17

0.656

48

2*

F

5

5

287

349

25

23

23

22

22

0.920

49

2

F

5

2

468

1006

25

22

22

10

2

0.648

50

2

F

5

4

511

482

15

23

23

12

2

0.600

51

2

F

5

4

649

229

0

21

21

12

5

0.472

52

2

F

5

5

231

229

15

24

24

11

11

0.680

53

2

F

5

5

227

212

15

21

21

14

13

0.672

54

2

F

5

3

265

258

15

23

23

13

10

0.672

55

2

F

5

2

668

Gave up

25

24

24

11

11

0.760

56

2

F

5

2

224

411

25

25

25

14

14

0.824

57

2*

F

5

5

280

204

15

25

25

21

21

0.856

58

2*

F

5

4

236

456

25

23

23

19

19

0.872

59

2*

F

5

3

236

456

25

23

23

17

17

0.840

60

2*

F

5

5

419

562

25

23

23

15

15

0.808

aParticipant, bVaie language, cMalay language, *part-time fishermen.

 

Language Priority and Retrieval of Information (Criteria A, B)

The Vaie people are generally proficient in more than one language. In addition to Vaie, they may also be adept in Melanau, Iban, Kedayan, or Malay, with the Malay being either standard Malay, Brunei Malay, or both (Edris and Ghani 1992; Ghani 2014). All 60 participants declared themselves as proficient in Vaie in the language proficiency questionnaire. Thirty-nine (65%) participants declared that they were “very good” in Malay, while eight (13%) participants stated that they were “good” in Malay (four from C1 and 4 from C2); ten participants (17%) regarded their proficiency in Malay to be “moderate” (six from C1 and four from C2), and three participants (5%) rated their language skills in Malay to be “poor” (all from C2). All participants who declared themselves not fully proficient in Malay were above 40 years old, indicating greater acquisition of Malay in the age group < 40. Three respondents (one from C1 and two from C2) were unable to complete the list of 25 fishes in Malay and withdrew from the survey after ten minutes. A weak correlation (r=-0.235) was found between self-assessed language proficiency and the time taken for free-listing in L1 and L2 (criteria A). This indicates that Vaie people are unaware of the loss of proficiency in L1, with their L2 gradually replacing L1. This is ascertained from the fact that people who self-assessed their L1 proficiency as “very good” had difficulty in free-listing fish names in L1, but had little difficulty in L2. Although the mean score for Criteria A that assessed adeptness in bilingualism is 21.7, the non-fishermen group (C2) had a noticeably lower score of 18.3 than the group (C1, 25.0) who practice fishing.  Perhaps this is the beginning of a language shift in the case of Vaie members who have moved away from their traditional occupation of fishing. However, both clusters returned similar scores for Criterion B indicating that participants exhibited a healthy trend in retrieving information in L1. This could also mean that any shift towards L2 happening on the ground can be reversed with appropriate interventions or measures.

Knowledge Erosion (Criterion C)

As understood from the average values, participants from both the clusters did well in this criterion (C1=23.6; C2=23.3) indicating that knowledge erosion is not a concern at this stage. However, it should be noted that the majority of the fishes in the culturally salient list (20/25) were identified by unanalyzable lexemes by the community (Table 1). This is a major drawback of the methodology noted during the course of study. Although unanalyzable lex­emes are a vital component of ethnotaxonomic systems, this criterion may not be reliable in situations where a large number of unanalyzable lexemes turn out to be salient.

Lexical Recognition (Criterion D)

All participants reported that they were able to relate positively to the visual stimuli comprising of 25 fish images. Participants from Cluster 1 had a higher mean score of 20.1, while those in Cluster 2 had a lower mean score of 16.6, indicating that individuals who followed the traditional occupation of fishing were more skilled in recognizing the species due to their constant interaction with the marine ecosystem and the diversity of fishes they came across. Our study also found noticeable differences between the mean scores of males (C1=21.9, C2=17.8) and females (C1=18.3, C2=15.1). This phenomenon could be attributed to the fact that Vaie women culturally play a supportive role in fishing, though it is predominantly the men who carry out the fishing activities. The inability to recognize visuals may not always correspond to lack of knowledge, as lack of familiarity with the medium (Case et al. 2006), or lack of ability to feel the specimens (Wester and Yongvanit 2006), could also influence the ability to recognize specimens.  Our interviews also showed that Vaie TK is gender sensitive with men specializing in areas such as the ecology and morphology of fishes while women are the custodians of knowledge related to the processing and grading of fish, recipes, and folklore.

Social Support for Exchange of TK (Criterion E)

Of the total sample pool (C1 and C2), 65% reported that their parents were the primary source from which they had acquired TK. However, a cluster-wise analysis shows that all participants of the fishermen group (C1) had acquired their knowledge on fish (primarily) from parents and grandparents. In the non-fishermen (C2) group, 53% of participants stated that they had acquired knowledge from the local markets where they procure fish, 31% from parents and grandparents, 3% from media, 2% from books and schools, and 11% from friends. This shows that for the community members who had experienced occupational shift, the market had become the main source of knowledge of fishes, though parents and family members continued to impart TK.

The surveys undertaken at the three main markets of Bintulu (Pasar Utama Bintulu, Pasar Kampung Baru, and Pasar ABF) show that Malay, Melanau, Vaie, Chinese, Iban, and Bahasa Indonesia are the most frequently used trade languages depending on the ethnicity of the traders and consumers. It is known that people who migrate from various places, with different sets of TK and skills, adapt to new ecosystems while influencing each other (van Andel et al. 2014). This feature was also observable at the markets studied, where vendors traded fish along with the knowledge and lexemes connected to them. Two phenomena directly influencing TK and language noted in this study are: 1) the grouping of fishes and 2) the modification of fish names based on the market language.

The grouping of fish is the clustering of fishes in the market for trade purposes. At these local markets, the terms satu tompok (one bundle), ikan campur (mixed fish) and ikan satu Malaysia (One Malaysia Fish) are used to group multiple fishes. Thus, satu tompok is a simple cluster of otherwise unrelated fishes put together in a plate, bucket, or simply heaped and sold together, while ikan satu Malaysia is another group of unrelated fishes named after the popular “One Malaysia” campaign of Malaysia’s Prime Minister that em­phasizes ethnic harmony, national unity, and efficient governance. Consumers who buy these groups of fishes gradually develop a cognitive notion that these categories of fishes are related to each other. An example of a modified name is “ikan proton saga” meaning “Proton Saga fish”, used to group four different species such as Osteogeneiosus militaris, Nemapteryx macronotacantha, Arius sp., and Arius maculatus. Proton Saga is a popular car brand, produced by Proton Malaysia Ltd., and fishes generally grouped under this name tend to have a larger head profile with a black-silver color, reminding people of the car brand. The Vaie names for these fishes are njen buleng, njen taoq, njen gagog and njenbageng respectively. These two phenomena show how markets influence TK and language in the non-fishermen group (C2).  An analysis of how markets contribute to the distortion of traditional knowledge related to fish names showed that markets were the source of 58% of inaccurate knowledge (modification/substitution of local folk names with non-local ones), and 53% of the correct knowledge related to fish names and identification. Additionally, markets have also replaced 31% of fish names with names from languages other than Vaie.

Of the total participants of both clusters, 97% (58 people) were married adults who reported active transmitting of fish knowledge to their children. Participants from C2 reported their inability to acquire TK from the formal schooling they had undergone, and 20 participants of this cluster were of the view that the TraLaVi assessment helped them realize the limitations they had in terms of depth of Vaie TK. Interestingly, only 10% of the 60 respondents reported acquiring knowledge from folklore and taboos (C1 n=5 individuals, C2 n=1 individual). Our baseline study documented very little information on folklore and taboos, indicating that a significant portion of this segment of TK and language may irrevocably be lost. The results for this criterion show that social interaction had been taking place, albeit at different levels, that are specific to the clusters. In the fishermen group, the traditional route of TK acquisition and transmission largely prevailed, while in the non-fishermen group, the market provided the main platform for knowledge acquisition.  Although rural markets are referred to as important sites for social interaction and exchange of knowledge (Tumbuan et al. 2006; Watson and Studdert 2006), they open up greater possibilities of external knowledge infusion into the community, in the absence of community-driven conservation efforts.

Assessing Vaie Language Vitality Using UNESCO’s LVE Framework

Table 3 provides the summarized results of the Language Vitality assessment using UNESCO’s Language Vitality and Endangerment index (LVE). The results show that the language vitality scenario of the Vaie language can be considered as largely “unsafe” despite the high language pride exhibited by the community. The results of LVE indicates that intergenerational transmission, number of speakers, trends in language usage, response to new domains and education, policy support, and documentation are not on the side of the language and urgent intervention measures are required.

 

Table 3Results of the Language Vitality and Endangerment index (UNESCO 2003).

Factor

Degree

Statement

1. Intergenerational Language Transmission

4 (Unsafe)

All sixty participants declared that they speak Vaie and are also involved in transmitting it to the younger generation. Vaie children and families speak Vaie as L1. However, our inter­views show that the language usage is confined exclusively to the domains’ family and peer group (Vaie to Vaie), and transmitted incompletely between generations. Thus, the Vaie language falls into the Unsafe (rating: 4) category, as suggested by the LVE.

2. Absolute Number of Speakers

18,000 (Unsafe)

Spoken by around 18,000 individuals, Vaie can be categorized as a small language community.

3. Proportion of Speakers within the Total Population

4 (Unsafe)

Three participants reported that 60% of the community speak Vaie, another three people reported 70%, 21 reported 75%, 11 reported 80%, 10 reported (85%), four reported 90%, and seven reported 95%. The maximum percentage of 100% was declared by just one participant. The mean value of 80% could be considered as the proportion of Vaie speakers to the total Vaie population, and can thus be categorized as “unsafe”. 

4. Trends in Existing Language Domain

4 (Multilingu-al parity)

1. Malay becomes the primary language for official purposes

2. Vaie is used in social domains

3.Vaie people are bilinguals

4. The Vaie believe that Malay is the language of social and economic opportunity

5. Response to New Domain and Media

1 (Minimal)

The Vaie language is used only in a few new domains. Of the 60 participants, 43 were of the view that Vaie is used only in a few new domains and 17 stated that there were no new domains where Vaie is applied. Although we see the beginning of a progressive upward trend here, as we came across a few websites, blogs, social media pages using Vaie, we provide a rating of “1” indicating a long way ahead.

6. Material for Language Education and Literacy

1 (Little material)

Vaie orthography is known to the community and some material is being written.
We could document only two books and a dictionary in Vaie. Participants were of the view that little educational materials are available in Vaie despite the earnest efforts of a few community members. Thus, we assign a score of “1”, indicating that little materials are available to the community in L1.

7. Governmental and Institutional Language Attitudes and Policies, Including Official Status Use

4 (Differ­entiated support)

1. Malay is the national language

2. Government recognizes the Vaie language

3. The Vaie language is mostly confined to domestic and social domains

8. Community Members’ Attitudes toward Their Own Language

5 (High language pride)

Immense language pride; all participants were of the view that the Vaie language has to be promoted in all domains. Participants link the Vaie language with their identity, heritage and ecosystem.

9. Amount and Quality of Documentation

1

(Inadequate)

Only a few grammatical and short word lists exist; there are no known audio-visual recordings of Vaie. People widely hold the belief that Vaie is an orally transmitted language.

 

Conclusion

The study concludes that the Vaie language could be regarded as “safe” for the moment. However, closer scrutiny indicates that community members practicing traditional fishing demonstrate greater language and TK vitality than non-fishermen. Although the results for the non-fishermen group indicate a “safe” status, the results hover close to the “vulnerable” status, indicating the need for intervention. An important outcome of the TraLaVi approach is the insight into knowledge transmission patterns within the community, as understood from criterion E. This provides opportunities for planning precise intervention measures for sustaining the Vaie language and TK. As expected, TraLaVi provides an ecosystem level evaluation of the status of language and TK vitality. Nevertheless, it addresses only one part of the question and ignores the external factors influencing language and TK vitality. In this study, the external factors influencing language vitality have been established only by the application of the UNESCO index that deals exclusively with language vitality.  Other limitations noted during the course of study are: 1) tracing the origin of fish names. Our respondents often struggled to identify the loan words. In-depth interviews delving into the cultural background of the lexemes were helpful; a superficial application of the index would have been either insufficient or resulted in erroneous data.   2) Elaborate fieldwork is required at least during the initial stage to document information on fishes. The lead researcher was living with the community for the entire course of the study which helped in expediting data collection. 3) As noted in Franco et al. (2015), the TraLaVi approach may not be suitable for languages that are not ecosystem specific. In the Vaie context, both the language and the TK are ecosystem specific. With due consideration to the abovementioned limitations, the study shows that TraLaVi can be successfully applied on the field to assess language and TK vitality, so as to complement existing indices such as the LVE developed by UNESCO.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

We declare no conflict of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgements

We express our sincere gratitude to the Ministry of Education, Malaysia and the Firebird Foundation for Anthropological Research for the generous funding and support. Special thanks to Prof. George N. Appell of the Foundation for his constant support and encourage­ment. We also thank the State Planning Unit, Sarawak for permitting us to carry out this research. The fieldwork wouldn’t have been possible without timely help from Prof. Ian Kerr, former PVC of Curtin University, Sarawak Malaysia, Prof. Alvin W. Yeo of ISITI, Universiti Malaysia Sarawak, Prof. Yudi Samyudia, and Mr. Alex Hiang Boon Chung of Curtin University Sarawak. We thank the Bintulu District Fisheries Office and the Persatuan Nelayan Kawasan Bintulu for sharing valuable data and literature. Special thanks to Encik Bolhassan bin Ismail, Encik Mat bin Suai, Encik Kapeh bin Hosen, Encik Ibrahim bin Saad, Puan Sahanian binti Sulong, and all participants from the Vaie community for accepting our invitation for collaboration and for participating in this study. This paper has also benefitted from the discussions the first author held with Ms. Savitri Kurnia, formerly at Culture and Language Studies, Curtin University Malaysia, and the suggestions received from Dr. Kathrina bte DP Haji Mohd Daud of Universiti Brunei Darussalam.

Funding

This research received funding from the Ministry of Education, Malaysia through its FRGS scheme, and a fellowship awarded to Merlin Franco by the Firebird Foundation for Anthropological Research, USA.

References Cited

Alliance for Linguistic Diversity. Bintulu [web page]. URL: http://www.endangeredlanguages.com/lang/bny. Accessed on October 5, 2015.

Asmah, H.O. 1983. The Malay Peoples of Malaysia and Their Languages. Dewan Bahasa dan Pustaka, Kementerian Pelajaran, Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia.

Blust, R. A. 1974. The Proto-North Sarawak Vowel Deletion Hypothesis. The University of Hawaii Press, Honolulu, HI.

Bostoen, K. 2007. Bantu Plant Names as Indicators of Linguistic Stratigraphy in the Western Province of Zambia. In Selected Proceedings of the 37th Annual Conference on African Linguistics, edited by D. Payne, pp. 16–29. Cascadilla Press, Somerville, MA. 

Case, R.J., S. G. Franzblau, Y. Wang, S. H. Chob, D.D. Soejarto, and G.F. Pauli. 2006. Ethnopharmacological Evaluation of the Informant Consensus Model on Anti-Tuberculosis Claims among the Manus. Journal of Ethno-Pharmacology 106:82–89. DOI:10.1016/j.jep.2005.12.005.

Department of Statistics, Malaysia. 2010. Population and Housing Census of Malaysia. Department of Statistics, Putrajaya, Malaysia.

Donohue, M., and T. Denham. 2009. Banana (Musa spp.) Domestication in the Asia-Pacific Region: Linguistic and Archaeobotanical Perspectives. Ethnobotany Research & Applications 7:293–332. DOI:10.17348/era.7.0.293-332.

Edris, A., and B. A. A. Ghani. 1992. The Influences of the Sociocultural Environment and Other Languages on the Bintulu Dialect: An Overview. Sarawak Museum Journal 153:343–355.

Evans, N. 1997. Sign Metonymies and the Problem of Flora-Fauna Polysemy in Australian Linguistics. In Boundary Rider: Essays in Honour of Geoffrey O’ Grady, edited by D. Tryon and M. Walsh, pp.C–136:133–153. Pacific Linguistics, Canberra, Australia.

Fabunmi, F.A., and A.S. Salawu. 2005. Is Yorùbá an Endangered Language? Nordic Journal of African Studies 14:391–408.

Fishman, J.A. 1991. Reversing Language Shift. Multilingual Matters Ltd, Clevedon, UK.

Franco, F.M., S. Hidayati, B. A. A Ghani, and B. R. Malancon. 2015. Ethnotaxonomic Systems Can Reflect the Vitality Status of Indigenous Languages and Traditional Knowledge. Indian Journal of Traditional Knowledge 14:175–182.

Ghani, B. A. A. 1992. The Bintulu Dialect—An Introduction. Sarawak Museum Journal 153: 321–341.

Ghani, B. A. A. 2006. The Concept of Language Shift and Maintenance. In Essays in Honour of Professor James T. Collins: Reflection in Borneo Rivers, edited by S. Chong, K. Harun, Y. Alas and J. T. Collins, pp. 111–123. STAIN Pontianak Press, Indonesia.

Ghani, B. A. A. 2014. Bahasa dan Masyarakat Vaie. In Daftar Kata Bahasa Vaie-Bahasa Melayu, edited by A. Hjh Asmiah, W. Mohd Riduwan and A. Santrol, pp. xix–xxix. Dewan Bahasa & Pustaka, Sarawak, Malaysia.

Hunn, E. 2007. Ethnobiology in Four Phases. Journal of Ethnobiology 27:1–10. DOI:10.2993/0278-0771(2007)27[1:EIFP]2.0.CO;2.

Ibrahim, S. 1971.  Kisah Bintulu. Borneo Literature Bureau, Kicking, Malaysia.

International Society of Ethnobiology. 2006. The ISE code of ethics [webpage]. URL: http://ethnobiology.net/code-of-ethics/. Accessed on 9 May 2017.

Kakudidi, E.K. 2004. Folk Plant Classification by Communities around Kibale National Park, Western Uganda. African Journal of Ecology 42:57–63. DOI:10.1111/j.1365-2028.2004.00462.x

Khiok, A. L. P., and A. C. Gambang. 2009. Field Guide to Marine & Estuarine Fishes of Sarawak. Fisheries Research Institute Bintawa, Kuching, Malaysia.

Khiok, A. L. P., and A. Ali. 2014. Marine Fishes Identification Sheet. Department of Fisheries Malaysia, Ministry of Agriculture and Agro-Based Industry, Malaysia.

Krauss, M. 1992. The World’s Language in Crisis. Language 68:4–10. DOI:10.1353/lan.1992.0075.

Luborsky, M.R., and R. L. Rubinstein.1995. Sampling in Qualitative Research. Research on Aging 17:89–113. DOI:10.1177/0164027595171005.

 Rangan, H., K. L. Bell, D. A. Baum, R. Fowler, P. McConvell, T. Saunders, S. Spronck, C. A. Kull, and D. J. Murphy. 2015. New Genetic and Linguistic Analyses Show Ancient Human Influence on Baobab Evolution and Distribution in Australia. PLoS ONE 10: e0127582. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0127582.

Simons, G.F., and C. D. Fennig, eds. 2017. Ethnologue: Languages of the World, 20th edition. SIL International, Dallas, Texas [website] URL: http://www.ethnologue.com. Accessed on May 09, 2017.

Tumbuan, A., L. Kawet, and Y. Shiratake. 2006. Significance of Traditional Market and Supermarket Functions for Local Farmers and Consumers: A Case Study on Marketing of Vegetables in Manado, North Sulawesi, Indonesia. Bulletin of the Faculty of Agriculture Saga 92:79–94.

Turpin M. 2013. Semantic Extension in Kaytetye Flora and Fauna Terms. Australian Journal of Linguistics 33:488-518. DOI:10.1080/07268602.2013.857571

Unasho, A. 2013. Language as Genes of Culture and Biodiversity Conservation: The Case of “Zaysite” Language in Southern region of Ethiopia. International Journal of Modern Anthropology 6:13–36. DOI:10.4314/ijma.v1i6.1.

UNESCO Ad Hoc Expert Group on Endangered Languages. 2003. Language Vitality and Endangerment. International Expert Meeting on UNESCO Programme Safeguarding of Endangered Languages Paris, 10–12 March 2003.

van Andel, T.R., C. I. E. A. van ‘t Kloosterc, D. Quiroz, A. M. Towns, S. Ruysschaert, and M. van den Berg. 2014. Local Plant Names Reveal that Enslaved Africans Recognized Substantial Parts of the New World Flora. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 111:E5346–E5353. DOI:10.1073/pnas.1418836111

Watson, S., and D. Studdert. 2006. Markets as Sites for Social Interaction Spaces of Diversity. The Policy Press, Great Britain.

Wester, L., and S. Yongvanit. 2006. Naming Consistency for Forest Plants in Some Rural Communities of Northeast Thailand. Ethnobotany Research and Applications 4:203–212.

Zaini, O. 1989. Bahasa Melanau: Satu Tanggapan Awal. The Sarawak Museum Journal 61: 231–250.

Zariquiey, R. 2014. Name Types, Polysemy, and Contrast Sets in Kakataibo Ethnobiological Nomenclature (Pano, Peru). Journal of Ethnobiology 34:251–272. DOI:10.2993/0278-0771-34.2.251.